Conventional wisdom asserts that drugs of abuse have
different patterns of action and that a drug of choice among abusers is the
drug that best fits abusers’ preferred course of action (Khantzian &
Shaffer, 1981; Mathias, 2001; Shaffer & Simoneau, 2001). However, empirical evidence about the matter
also indicates that an abuser’s drug of use also depends on its ease of access,
ingestion, and surrounding social circumstances (Harford, 1978). In this issue of STASH, we
discuss Newton, De La Garza II, Kalechstein, and Nestor’s (2005) comparison of the subjective
and physiologic effects of cocaine and methamphetamine.
Figure 1: Cardiovascular and Subjective Effects: Methamphetamine vs. Cocaine (Adapted from: Newton et al, (2005))
This study has some limitations. First, the cocaine and methamphetamine groups might not be representative of the cocaine and methamphetamine dependent populations. (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2007a, 2007b; Winslow, 2007). Second, researchers administered both drugs intravenously, which can result in a different rate of absorption than other routes of administration for these users. Users often have several routes of administration (e.g., oral, inhaled, intravenous). Third, laboratory settings are very different from the in vivo contexts within which users administrate their drugs. Research has shown that the social setting influences subjective state more than most observers recognize (Zinberg, 1984).
Despite these limitations, the mean heart rate changes and the participants’ descriptions of subjective effects indicate a correlation between substance user perceptions, drug induced physiological effects, and the need that these circumstances create for drug use. An understanding of the preferred type or attributes of drug effect could help researchers and practitioners to better redirect craving and find healthier substitutes. For example, a methamphetamine user could redirect addictive tendencies towards another stimulating activity (e.g., cross country running), which somewhat mimics, but is healthier, than stimulant use.
What do you think? Comments can be addressed to Ingrid R. Maurice
References
Khantzian, E. J. (1975). Self selection and progression in
drug dependence. Psychiatry Digest, 36, 19-22.
Khantzian, E. J. (1997). The self-medication hypothesis of
substance use disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard
Review of Psychiatry, 4(5), 231-244.
Mathias, R. (2001). Even Modest Cocaine Use May Cause Brain
Changes That Could Contribute to Addiction. NIDA Notes, 16(3).
Newton, T. F.,
De La Garza II, R., Kalechstein, A.D. , Nestor, L. (2005). Cocaine and methamphetamine
produce different patterns of subjective and cardiovascular effects.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 82(1), 90-97.
Simon, S. L., Richardson, K., Dacey, J, Glynn, S, Domier,
C.P., Rawson, R.A., et al. (2002). A comparison of patterns of methamphetamine
and cocaine use. Journal Addictive Disorders, 21(1), 35-44.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.
(2007a). The NSDUH Report: Demographic and Geographic Variations in Injection
Drug Use.Rockville.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.
(2007b). The NSDUH Report: Worker Substance Use, by Industry Category.Rockville.
Winslow, B. T., Voorhees, K.I, Pehl K.A. (2007). Methamphetamine
Abuse. American Family Physician, 76(8), 1169-1174.
Zinberg, N. E. (1984). Drug, Set, and Setting: The Basis for Controlled Intoxicant Use. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Comments